# Introduction substantial body of literature shows that some persons can support beliefs that rape victims are responsible for their own fate. Stereotyped and prejudicial beliefs about rape and the victims and perpetrators can contribute to sexual violence by shifting blame for the assault from the perpetrator to the victim (Jain, 2011; Kamdar, Kosambiya, Chawada, Verma, & Kadia, 2017).Cultural, institutional, and personal aspects can further promote such equivocal judgment of the victims (Maddux & Yuki, 2006). Although rape myth acceptance can be a product of psychosocial aspects, it is also a response to messages from social, family, media, and groups that propagate the legitimacy of such myths. This study tests a correlational model involving some factors possibly related to rape myth acceptance. If an individual holds stereotypical or false beliefs about rape and the victims and perpetrators, he or she will likely have a negative attitude toward sexual assault survivors. Although institutional, cultural, and individual aspects are specific fields of knowledge and research, it is not possible to disentangle one from another. For example, culture is the umbrella under which both social behaviors and individual beliefs exist. The world is full of unexploited psychological variations that offer a broad scope to develop and better test different theories (Henrich, 2015). In Brazil, for example, "the cultural structure of the nature of the sexual realities and the interpretation of the meanings of the sexual practices have been based on the distinctions between masculinity and femininity, activity and passivity, domination and submission, and so on (?). These distinctive roles are gradually internalized through a complex process of socialization beginning in the earliest moments of childhood" (Parker, 1991). Also, "traditionally, men gain respect by performing the role of the 'macho', that is, those with a virile and brave status. Thus, a man should demonstrate his virility, measured by the number of sexual conquests" (Hayes, 2011). It should be noted that we performed this study in a culture held up by a patriarchal structure, where there is socially demarcated roles of men and women, as well as learned beliefs. However, individuals may choose to act or think in ways that are contrary to the current cultural expectancies (Dressler, Balieiro, & Dos Santos, 1997). Regarding rape myth acceptance, more men than women seem to blame the female victims for the assault (Basow & Minieri, 2011;Struckman-Johnson, 1992;Walfield, 2018); no study has suggested that women engage in such beliefs more frequently than men (Gravelin, Biernat, & Bucher, 2018). This is not surprising considering rape is mainly, if not exclusively, women's concern, and sexist cultural views still predominate in many different societies (Prado Cortez, Boer, & Baltieri, 2011; Stoll, Lilley, & Pinter, 2017). There are, however, studies that show that there were no significant differences between men and women about blaming the victim (Abrams, Viki, Masser, & Bohner, 2003;Persson, Dhingra, & Grogan, 2018). Sexism, mainly the hostile kind that comprises negative attitudes toward individuals who violate traditional gender A stereotypes, seems to be a common denominator of rape myth acceptance (Chapleau, 2007) and can be associated with risky sexual behaviors (Glick, 1996). On the contrary, being non-heteronormative has been negatively correlated with rape myth acceptance (Worthen, 2017). More women are victims of sexual assault than men (World Health Organization, 2003), and they experience a wide range of emotions such as shame, guilt, depression, and denial as a response to the traumatic event. Some victims even blame themselves for it, confusing their participation in the assault (McLindon, 2011). In a male-dominated society, women may accept the blame for their own victimization "because they are the gatekeepers in sexual interactions" (Bridges, 1991). Although it is possible that previous victimization and cultural expectations can influence women' beliefs, this assertion cannot be generalized (Carmody, 2001). Highly educated women may not condone these sexist statements. Some studies show a significant correlation between depression symptoms and rape myth acceptance among female victims (Arata, 1999;Feiring, Taska, & Chen, 2002). Other ones have shown a correlation between depression and justice sensitivity. In reality, in situations where a justice-sensitive individual is simply an observer, he or she tends to wish compensation for the victims and punishment for the perpetrators. Higher rape myth acceptance scores seem to be a factor in risky sexual behaviors (Kalichman et al., 2005). Also, some studies have demonstrated that male aggression against women and inconsistent condom use may be related and that some men can use sexual tactics to ensure unprotected sex (Davis, 2010; Davis & Logan-Greene, 2012). Among men, high score on hostile sexism seems to be linked to lower condom use (Ramiro-Sanchez, Ramiro, Bermudez, & Buela-Casal, 2018). Individuals engaging in inconsistent condom use could be solely focusing on the reward of sex than the consequences of such conduct . Furthermore, issues of power, control over interaction with sexual partners, and the use of psychoactive substances are factors associated with pernicious sexual practices (Parsons, Bimbi, Koken, & Halkitis, 2005). Also, sensation-seeking, high impulsiveness, and risk-taking are linked to risky sexual practices (Kalichman, Heckman, & Kelly, 1996). Thus, inconsistent condom use can be an indicator of impulsiveness and transgression (Halkitis & Parsons, 2003). Alcohol and drugs are common elements in rape cases, especially those that occur on university campuses (Abbey, 1996). Many studies have shown that intoxicated victims are blamed more often for the assault than those who were sober; in contrast, the drunker the perpetrator, the more excuses he gives for his behavior (Qi, 2016;Richardson, 1992). Although it is crucial to investigate how different segments of the population endorse rape myths, university students comprise a relevant group because they are at high risk of facing sexual assault (Daigle, 2013). Considering this, we hypothesized the following: being a heterosexual male, using drugs and inconsistent condom use can act as supportive factors for rape myth acceptance; in contrast, considering a sample of welleducated participants, a history of being sexually abused in childhood, depression scores, and being non-heteronormative do not. # II. # Method a) Procedure We performed a cross-sectional study to investigate correlations between some psychosocial variables, mainly those potentially related to rape myths, such as biological sex, sexual orientation, drug use, and depression. The investigators were specially trained medical graduate and postgraduate medical students. The Ethics Committee of ABC Medical School, Santo André, São Paulo, Brazil approved this study. # b) Participants Between September 2016 and August 2019, a total of 280 medical students aged 18 and above, from the first through the sixth year at the medical school, were randomly selected and recruited to participate in this study. The researchers assured participants that only the researchers would see the data, that all data would be kept confidential, and that the partaking was voluntary. Brazilian law does not allow financial compensation for participants of any researches. Participant-important outcomes were compared based on 13 variables: biological sex, age, race, marital status, lifetime alcohol use, lifetime drug use (mainly marijuana), family members with alcohol/drug use problems, sexual abuse history, inconsistent condom use, sexual orientation, and scores of depression symptoms, drug use severity, and rape myth acceptance. We codified sex as male, female, and intersex. We did not codify monthly income as our participants were pursuing a full-time course. # c) Measures The participants provided information through a self-reported questionnaire, which included items assessing sociodemographic characteristics and the following: the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST), and Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance (IRMA). # The Beck Depression Inventory This 21-item inventory measures behavioral responses related to depression among adults and adolescents. Scores above 10 (in the range 0-63) indicate the presence of a depressive syndrome ( # The Drug Abuse Screening Test The DAST is a quantitative self-report instrument used to detect drug misuse among a range of psychoactive drugs. The original version contains 28 yes/no questions; a cutoff score of ? 6 (total score range 0-28) indicates a probable drug-use problem (Gavin, Ross, & Skinner, 1989). A cutoff score of 6 or 7 has a sensitivity of 0.96 and a specificity of 0.85. The DAST has been translated into Portuguese for administration to Brazilian adults (Baltieri & Andrade, 2008). # The Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Sgrillo Scarpati (2014) validated The Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale -Short form (IRMA) (McMahon, 2011), an update of the 1999 version of the IRMA (Payne, 1999),in a sample of university students in Brazil. This instrument consists of 22-item scale that includes statements on a woman and man's role in a sexual situation that may lead to assault; it addresses women as the victims and men as the perpetrators. Participants indicate how much they agree with each statement on a 5 point scale from strongly agree to strongly disagree. Higher scores indicate more rejection of rape myths. This instrument has four factors: Factor 1 or "She asked for it" has statements such as "If a woman is raped while she is drunk, she is at least somewhat responsible for letting things get out of control" and "When women go around wearing low-cut or short skirts, they are just asking for trouble"; Factor 2 or "He did not have intention" has statements such as "When men rape, it is because of their strong desire for sex" and "Men did not usually intend to force sex on a woman, but sometimes they get too sexually carried away"; Factor 3 or "It was not really a rape" has statements such as "If a woman does not physically resist sex, it really cannot be considered rape" and "If the rapist does not have a weapon, you really cannot call it a rape"; and Factor 4 or "She lied" has statements such as: "Rape accusations are often used as a way of getting back at men" and "Women who are caught having an illicit affair sometimes claim that it was rape". The global internal consistency of this validated instrument was ? = 0.72, and the internal consistencies of the 4 factors were 0.74, 0.64, 0.68, and 0.71, respectively. The factor "He did not have intention" showed low internal consistency, which may mean a poor correlation between each item of this factor. # d) Analysis Univariate analyses were used to compare the sociodemographic and psychometric factors between the male and female participants. Categorical variables were compared using the ? 2 or Fisher's exact tests, following the Monte Carlo method. Continuous variables were compared using student's t-test. To develop a correlational model, we performed structured equation modeling (SEM). Maximum likelihood estimation was used to estimate the fit of the model. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), Goodness of Fit Index (GFI), Adjusted GFI (AGFI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR) were used to evaluate the model fit. Some recommendations regarding the values for global model fit were adopted ? specifically, CFI, TLI, GFI, and AGFI values greater than 0.90, and RMSEA and SRMR values lower than 0.08 were deemed indicative of an acceptable model fit (Gilson et al., 2013;Hu & Bentler, 1999). As the chi-square value is dependent on the sample size, we calculated the ratio of chi-square relative to the degrees of freedom (? 2 /df), where a value of 2 or lower is an acceptable ? 2 /df ratio (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). # III. # Results Of the returned questionnaires, 11 (3.93%) were discarded due to incomplete answers, leaving 269 participants. Of these, 175 (65.06%) were female, and the mean age of the total sample was 21.35 (SD = 2.51) years. Our sample did not have intersex or transgender participants. The global internal consistency of the IRMA for this sample was ? = 0.70, and the internal consistencies of the 4 factors were 0.72, 0.62, 0.72, and 0.73, respectively. Again, Factor 2 (He did not have intention) show low internal consistency, which is in line with the validated version in Brazil. # Descriptive Analysis In Table 1, we decided to compare the male and female participants in terms of sociodemographic and psychosocial factors because a large number of studies have shown higher rape myth acceptance among men than women. In fact, the male participants did show significantly higher rape myth acceptance for all factors than their counterparts. With respect to other variables, there were no significant differences between the male and female participants in terms of mean age, race, marital status, inconsistent condom use, lifetime alcohol use, lifetime drug use, family history of alcohol/drug use problems, being non-heteronormative, and DAST scores. However, female participants had higher BDI scores as well as prevalence of sexual abuse history. Given that the variable "male" was strongly associated with all factors of the IRMA, and that there were significant differences between the male and female participants in two other variables, we decided not to include it in the SEM to avoid weakening the influence of the hypothesized variables on rape myth acceptance. # SEM Analysis Items were loaded uniquely on their relevant factors, and the factor loadings were fixed at 1.0. Then, the sample was evaluated using bootstrapping (400 bootstrap samples) with the Bollen-Stine Bootstrap statistic being conducted to verify absolute fit. As it is shown in Figure 1, the model fitted the data well, with # Discussion This study supports previous research that being male and using drugs are supportive factors for blaming the victim, while being non-heteronormative is not. However, holding up our initial hypotheses but somewhat disagreeing with other studies, depression levels and sexual abuse history did not support rape myth acceptance. Each factor of the IRMA assesses a group of myths that function to neutralize the perpetrator's responsibility. However, the Factor "It wasn't really rape" does not properly include a victim-blaming or responsibility-denying component (Mouilso, 2013); it seems to be a subtle construct of victim-blaming. This factor simply presumes that a woman consented to a sexual relationship because she did not display any kind of a physical reaction against the act, neither presumably "instigated" male sexual instincts. Participants with higher depression scores could have perceived this and reacted more strongly. In fact, some studies have shown that depressive individuals seem to react more intensely to injustice and threats of rejection (Schmitt, 2005). In addition, having a low threshold for injustice can make sensitive individuals recognize more cases of it than those who are insensitive. The myths transferring responsibility onto the victims (Factors 1 and 4) positively correlated with higher drug use scores but negatively with being nonheteronormative. Youth substance users can attribute their own responsibilities to others. More recently, the perception of drug users as rational pleasure-seekers has legitimated management focused on individual responsibility; it has been important to transform young people into self-reflexive and responsible citizens (Ekendahl, 2020). Several people believe that intoxicated men can misinterpret different behaviors, including women's. Despite these beliefs, our study suggests that even sober drug users can maintain these misconceptions. Drug users may demonstrate interpersonal problems and impaired or distorted perceptions of emotions in others. In fact, deficits in social processing can persist even after long periods of abstinence (Kornreich et al., 2001;Miller, Bershad, & de Wit, 2015;Preller et al., 2014). Studies have also suggested that nonheteronormative individuals are less likely to support Although comparisons between the male and female participants in terms of depression symptoms and sexual abuse history were not the main goal of this study, the higher mean scores on depression in women than in men, mainly among young people (Albert, Furthermore, as Figure 1 shows, where the statistically significant correlations are visualized, only higher DAST scores supported rape myth acceptance; sexual abuse history, being non-heteronormative, and higher depression scores did not. Inconsistent condom use did not correlate with rape myth acceptance but positively correlated with sexual abuse history. Factor 2 (He did not have intention) did not correlate with any variables included in SEM; therefore, it was visually suppressed with the aim of making the figure cleaner. rape myths than heterosexual men and women (Worthen, 2017); reasons presented to explain this finding include the former being raped more frequently (Balsam, Rothblum, & Beauchaine, 2005), being less sexist, adhering less to patriarchal gender norms, and supporting feministic ideas more commonly than the latter. However, not all non-heteronormative individuals abandon patriarchal and cultural norms. For example, lesbians can have a certain animosity toward heterosexual women and support rape myths (Brewster, 2010); this can also be true to some extent vis-à-vis bisexuals and transsexuals. Although individuals who have suffered sexual abuse can feel confused about their role in the assault, recent social movements, such as #Me Too, have emphasized the need to raise awareness about the prevalence and harms of such acts; survivors have started rejecting more strongly the societal norms that previously demanded their silence or even acceptance (AcAdam, 1999). This system of false beliefs regarding victim-blaming where "victims have been denigrated for their role in the rape, even to the extent whereby the victim is held responsible for the assault" (Grubb, 2012; Tyson, 2019) has been vehemently challenged. Thus, the survivors have manifested their desire to give voice to their injuries. Therefore, it is important to listen to the victims because "they did not lie". As the results show, inconsistent condom use did not correlate with any factors of the IRMA but did with sexual abuse history. Sexual or physical abuse during childhood (Hamburger et al., 2004), use of psychoactive drugs, and personality-related factors (such as reward dependence) have been associated with the inconsistent condom use (Cortez et al., 2011). Different researchers have already correlated unprotected sexual practices with search for higher physical stimulation, attempts to cope with emotional fatigue and stress, a chaotic lifestyle, and alcohol and drug abuse (Kalichman et al., 1996;Kelly, Bimbi, Izienicki, & Parsons, 2009). Moreover, with the exception of being male, no other variable tested was correlated with the factor "He did not have intention". It is possible that the factor's low internal consistency in our sample affected its significant correlation with other variables. Nonetheless, this factor assumes that men have a great need for sex due to a presumable higher sexual impulsivity. We also believe that in a group of medical students who have the privilege of maintaining contact with different individuals on a university campus as well as with patients belonging to diverse cultural and social backgrounds, a reduction in prejudice and misunderstandings is possible and extremely positive (Taschler, 2017). Rape myths are still supported mainly by men and drug users. What, however, can be considered as positive is that some variables that previously correlated with these myths, such as depression and sexual abuse history, did not in our study. Although we still live in a patriarchal society, university students can be encouraging to question their personal and sexual roles, and recreate our culture. To cultivate an inclusive and unbiased environment, it is important that no victim be pressured into taking responsibility for a sexual assault and reducing the blame on the perpetrator. Women combating sexual stereotypes represent freedom and dignity, which are important in a fair society. This study has several limitations that must be listed: a) The path model limits the interpretation of the results; although it shows directionality of the relationships, it is unable to establish causality between the variables b) Some individual (personal) variables are not evaluated in the correlational model, such as sex and race c) This study does not take into account institutional and situational factors possibly related to the rape myth acceptance (Gravelin et al., 2018); d) The IRMA entails a general tendency toward victimblaming, which may have affected the reliability of the findings (Dawtry, Cozzolino, & Callan, 2019) e) An evaluation of social desirability is suitable to increase the reliability of these findings. # Compliance with Ethical Standards Potential conflicts of interest: None. Authors have not received any grants. Ethical Approval: All procedures performed in the participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the ABC Medical School. Informed Consent: All participants signed a consent form to partake in this study. Constraints on publishing: There are no any constraints. ![Correlational Model of Rape Myth Acceptance and Psychosocial Factors among Medical School Students](image-2.png "A") A Correlational Model of Rape Myth Acceptance and Psychosocial Factors among Medical SchoolStudentsRial, & Rickels, 1974; Furlanetto, Mendlowicz, & RomildoBueno, 2005). This test has a sensitivity of 100% and aspecificity of 0.83 with a cut-off score of 9/10.Volume XX Issue X Version ID D D D ) K(Medical Research© 2020 Global Journals 1VariablesMale Students (n = 94)Female Students (n = 175)testpAge, mean (SD)21.46 (2.16)21.29 (2.67)t = 0.54, 267df0.59Race, n (%)White82 (87.23)153 (87.43)Non-White12 (12.77)22 (12.57)? 2 < 0.01, 1df0.93Marital status, n (%)Single93 (98.94)174 (99.43)Married/Common-law1 (1.06)1 (0.57)? 2 = 0.20, 1df> 0.99Sexual abuse inchildhood, n (%)6 (6.38)26 (14.86)? 2 = 4.19, 1df0.04*Preservative use, n (%)Consistent use62 (65.96)109 (62.28)Inconsistent use32 (34.04)66 (37.72)? 2 = 0.36, 1 df0.55Lifetime alcohol use,n (%)83 (88.30)161 (92.00)? 2 = 0.99, 1df0.32Lifetime illicit drug use, n(%)43 (45.74)81 (46.29)? 2 < 0.01, 1 df> 0.99Family members withalcohol/drug useproblems, n (%)53 (56.38)100 (57.14)? 2 = 0.01, 1df0.90Non-heteronormativity,n (%)13 (13.83)15 (8.57)? 2 = 1.81, 1df0.18BDI, mean (SD)8.11 (8.54)10.42 (7.35)t = -2.32, 267df0.02*DAST, mean (SD)4.72 (3.69)5.18 (3.81)t = -0.94, 267df0.35IRMA, mean (SD)Factor 127.87 (2.49)29.05 (1.70)t = -4.21, 267df< 0.01**Factor 227.18 (3.86)28.14 (2.69)t =-2.38, 267df0.02*Factor 324.31 (1.43)25.75 (0.71)t = -3.28, 267df< 0.01**Factor 422.02 (3.75)23.81 (2.11)t = -4.99, 267df< 0.01**Note: BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; DAST, Drug Abuse Screening Test; IRMA, Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance; * p < 0.05; ** p< 0.01 * Alcohol and dating risk factors for sexual assault among college women ARAbbey LTMcduffie DMcauslan P Psychology of Women Quarterly 20 1996 * Perceptions of stranger and acquaintance rape: The role of benevolent and hostile sexism in victim blame and rape proclivity DAbrams GTViki BMasser GBohner Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 2003 * Political process and the development of black insurgency 1930-1970 DAcadam 1999 University of Chicago Press Chicago * Why is depression more prevalent in women PRAlbert Journal of Psychiatry & Neurosciences 40 2015 * Coping with rape: The roles of prior sexual abuse and attitudes of blame CMArata Journal of Interpersonal Violence 14 1999 * Victimization over the life span: A comparison of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and heterosexual siblings KFBalsam EDRothblum TPBeauchaine Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 73 2005 * Comparing serial and nonserial sexual offenders: Alcohol and street drug consumption, impulsiveness and history of sexual abuse DABaltieri AGAndrade Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria 30 2008 * You owe me": Effects of date cost, who pays, participant gender, and rape myth beliefs on perceptions of rape SABasow AMinieri Journal of Interpersonal Violence 26 2011 * Short form of depression inventory: Cross-validation ATBeck WYRial KRickels Psychological Reports 34 1974 * Perceived experiences of antibisexual prejudice: Instrument development and evaluation MBrewster BMoradi Journal of Counseling Psychology 57 2010 * Perceptions of date and stranger rape: A difference in sex role expectations and rapesupportive beliefs JBridges Sex Roles 24 1991 * Rape myth acceptance among college women DCCarmody LMWashington Journal of Interpersonal Violence 16 2001 * How ambivalent sexism toward women and men support rape myth acceptance KMChapleau DOswalds BLRussel Sex Roles 57 2007 * Psychosocial pathways to inconsistent condom use among male sex workers: Personality, drug misuse and criminality FCCortez DPBoer DABaltieri Sexual Health 8 2011 * Adolescent onset of the gender difference in lifetime rates of major depression: A theoretical model JMCyranowski EFrank EYoung MKShear Archives of General Psychiatry 57 2000 * Victimology: The essentials LEDaigle 2013 Thousand Oaks; Sage * The influence of alcohol expectancies and intoxication on men's aggressive unprotected sexual intentions KCDavis Experimental Clinical Psychopharmacology 18 2010 * Young men's use of aggressive tactics to avoid condom use: A test of a theoretical model KCDavis PLogan-Greene Social Work Research 36 2012 * I blame therefore it was: Rape myth acceptance, victim blaming, and memory reconstruction RJDawtry PJCozzolino MJCallan Personality and Social Psycholoy Bulletin 45 2019 * The cultural construction of social support in Brazil: associations with health outcomes WWDressler MCBalieiro JESantos Medicine, and Psychiatry 21 1997 Culture * Risk and responsibilization: Resistance and compliance in Swedish treatment for youth cannabis use MEkendahl JMânsson PKarlsson Drugs: Educational, Prevention and Policy 27 2020 * Trying to understand why horrible things happen: Attribution, shame, and symptom development following sexual abuse CFeiring LTaska KChen Child Maltreatment 7 2002 * Depression and C-reactive protein in US adults: Data from the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey DEFord TPErlinger Archives of Internal Medice 164 2004 * The validity of the Beck Depression Inventory-Short Form as a screening and diagnostic instrument for moderate and severe depression in medical inpatients LMFurlanetto MVMendlowicz JBueno Journal of Affective Disorders 86 2005 * Diagnostic validity of the drug abuse screening test in the assessment of DSM-III drug disorders DRGavin HERoss HASkinner British Journal of Addiction 84 1989 * Validation of the Drinking Motives Questionnaire (DMQ) in older adults KMGilson CBryant BBei AKomiti HJackson FJudd Addictive Behaviors 38 2013 * The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism PGlick STFiske Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 1996 * Blaming the victim of acquaintance rape: Individual, situational, and sociocultural factors CRGravelin MBiernat CEBucher Frontiers in Psychology 9 2422 2018 * Attribution of blame in rape cases: A review of the impact of rape myth acceptance, gender role conformity and substance use on victim blaming AGrubb TEmily Aggression and Violent Behavior 17 2012 * Intentional unsafe sex (barebacking) among HIV-positive gay men who seek sexual partners on the internet PNHalkitis JTParsons AIDS Care 15 2003 * Persistence of inconsistent condom use: Relation to abuse history and HIV serostatus MEHamburger JMoore LJKoenig DVlahov EESchoenbaum PSchuman KMayer AIDS and Behavior 8 2004 * Holy harlots: Femininity, sexuality, & black magic in Brazil KEHayes 2011 California University Press London * Culture and social behavior JHenrich Behavioral Sciences 3 2015 * Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives LHu PMBentler Structural Equation Modeling 6 1999 * Sexual attitudes and myths among medical and non medical students: An exploratory study DJain GDKoolwal SGehlot AAwasthi Journal of Mental Health and Human Behaviour 2 2011 * Sensation seeking as an explanation for the association between substance use and HIV-related risky sexual behavior SCKalichman THeckman JAKelly Archives of Sexual Behavior 25 1996 * Gender attitudes, sexual violence, and HIV/AIDS risks among men and women in Cape Town SCKalichman LCSimbayi MKaufman DCain CCherry SJooste VMathiti Journal of Sex Research 42 2005 * Rape: Is it a lifestyle or behavioral problem? ZNKamdar JKKosambiya BLChawada MVerma AKadia Indian Journal of Psychiatry 59 2017 * Stress and coping among HIV-positive barebackers BCKelly DSBimbi HIzienicki JTParsons AIDS and Behavior 13 2009 * Deficits in recognition of emotional facial expression are still present in alcoholics after mid-to long-term abstinence CKornreich SBlairy PPhilippot UHess XNoel EStreel Journal of Studies on Alcohol 62 2001 * The "ripple effect": Cultural differences in perceptions of the consequences of events WWMaddux MYuki Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32 2006 * Listening to mental health workers' experiences: Factors influencing their work with women who disclose sexual assault EMclindon LHarms International Journal of Mental Health Nursing 20 2011 * An updated measure for assessing subtle rape myths SMcmahon GFarmer Social Work Research 35 2 2011 * Drug effects on responses to emotional facial expressions: Recent findings MAMiller AKBershad HDe Wit Behavioural Pharmacology 26 2015 * The role of rape myth acceptance and psychopathy in sexual assault perpetration ERMouilso KSCalhoun Journal of Agression, Maltreatment & Trauma 22 2013 * Bodies, Pleasures, and Passions. Sexual Culture in Contemporary Brazil RGParker 1991 Beacon Press Boston * Factors related to childhood sexual abuse among gay/bisexual male Internet escorts JTParsons DSBimbi JAKoken PNHalkitis Journal of Child Sexuao Abuse 14 2005 * Rape myth acceptance: Exploration of its structure and its measurement using the Illinois Rape Myth Acceptance Scale DAPayne KALonsway LFFitzgerald Journal of Research in Personality 33 1999 * Attributions of victim blame in stranger and acquaintance rape: A quantitative study SPersson KDhingra SGrogan Journal of Clinical Nursing 27 2018 * A psychosocial study of male-to-female transgendered and male hustler sex workers in Sao Paulo FCPrado Cortez DPBoer DABaltieri Brazil. Archives of Sexual Behavior 40 2011 * Functional changes of the reward system underlie blunted response to social gaze in cocaine users KHPreller MHerdener LSchilbach PStampfli LMHulka MVonmoos Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences National Academy of Sciences 2014 111 Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't * Attributions of responsibility, blame and justifiability to a perpetrator and victim in an acquaintance rape scenario: The influence of Marijuana intoxication SJQi LCStarfelt KMWhite Journal of Sexual Aggression 22 2016 * Sexism and sexual risk behavior in adolescents: Gender differences TRamiro-Sanchez MTRamiro MPBermudez GBuela-Casal International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology 18 2018 * Alcohol and rape: The effect of alcohol on atributions of blame for rape DCRichardson LCampbell Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 8 1992 * Adaptação da escala de aceitação dos mitos do estupro: Evidências de validade ASagrillo Scarpati VMartins Guerra CN BDuarte Avaliação Psicológica 13 2014 * Justice sensitivity: Assessment and location in the personality space MSchmitt MGollwitzer JMaes DArbach European Journal of Psychological Assessment 21 2005 * Gender-Blind sexism and rape myth acceptance LCStoll TGLilley KPinter Violence Against Women 23 2017 * Acceptance of male in America: New data challenge old assumptions CStruckman-Johnson DStruckman-Johnson Sex Roles 27 1992 * Using Multivariate Statistics BGTabachnick LSFidell 2007 Pearson Education Boston, MA * Contact with counter-stereotypical women predicts less sexism MTaschler KWest 2017